英文【ACT】如何ACT數學成績與課堂智能手機干擾和數學焦慮相關_第1頁
英文【ACT】如何ACT數學成績與課堂智能手機干擾和數學焦慮相關_第2頁
英文【ACT】如何ACT數學成績與課堂智能手機干擾和數學焦慮相關_第3頁
英文【ACT】如何ACT數學成績與課堂智能手機干擾和數學焦慮相關_第4頁
英文【ACT】如何ACT數學成績與課堂智能手機干擾和數學焦慮相關_第5頁
已閱讀5頁,還剩54頁未讀, 繼續免費閱讀

下載本文檔

版權說明:本文檔由用戶提供并上傳,收益歸屬內容提供方,若內容存在侵權,請進行舉報或認領

文檔簡介

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20251

HowACTMathematicsPerformanceIsRelatedtoClassroomSmartphoneDistractionsand

MathematicsAnxiety

JeffSchiel

Summary

Recentresultsfromlarge-scaletestingprograms(e.g.,theACT?test,Programmefor

InternationalStudentAssessment)showdeclinesovertimeinhighschoolstudents’

mathematicsperformance.Suchdeclinescanbeconcerningformany,includingstudents,

parents,andeducators.Severalfactorscouldberelatedtomathematicsperformancedeclines,includingtheCOVID-19pandemic,technologydistractionsinmathematicsclasses,and

mathematicsanxiety.ThisstudyinvestigatedtherelationshipbetweenACTmathematicsscoresandtwofactors:distractionsfromsmartphonesinmathematicsclassesandmathematics

anxiety.

Keyfindingsfromthestudyaredescribedbrieflybelow.

Distractionsfromsmartphonesinmathematicsclasses—whetherfromastudent’sownsmartphoneorfromthesmartphonesofotherstudents—werefoundtohaveasignificantnegativerelationshipwithsubsequentperformanceontheACTmathematicstest,evenwhenthestudycontrolledforimportantstudentbackgroundvariables(e.g.,gender,

race/ethnicity,highschoolGPA,numberofhighschoolmathematicscoursestaken).

MathematicslearninganxietyandmathematicsevaluationanxietywerebothsignificantlyandnegativelyrelatedtoACTmathematicsperformance.Thiswasevidentevenwhenstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.

Mathematicsisnottheonlysubjectforwhichdomain-specificanxietymightbeofconcern.Performanceinscience,asmeasuredbytheACTsciencetest,wasalsofoundtobe

significantlyandnegativelyrelatedtosciencelearninganxiety.

Introduction

Studentperformanceinmathematicsappearstohavedeclinedinthepastfewyears.Some

evidenceofthiscomesfromthe2022ProgrammeforInternationalStudentAssessment(PISA)assessment,whichwasadministeredtonearly700,00015-year-oldstudentsin81countriestoassessperformanceinreading,science,andespeciallymathematics.Itwasfoundthatthe

averagemathematicsscorehaddecreasedbyalmost15pointssincethelastassessment,

whichwasin2018.Thisdecreaseisunprecedented;until2022,nochangeintheaveragehaseverexceeded4points(OECD,2023).

CT

?2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20252

CT

?2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435

IntheirreportofthePISAresults,theOrganisationforEconomicCo-operationand

DevelopmentstatedthatthedropinmathematicsscorepointstotheshockeffectofCOVID-19onmostcountries(OECD,2023,p.27).Theyalsostated,however,thatthepandemicmightnotbetheonlyfactorinvolved,becausescienceperformancedidnotchangesignificantly,onaverage,between2018and2022.TheaveragePISAsciencescoredecreasedbyonly2pointsduringthisperiod.

Anotherhypothesisforthedeclineinmathematicsperformancewasthattechnologyis

distractingteenagersduringtheirmathematicsclasses.PISAfoundthatstudentswhoreportedbecomingdistractedinmostorallmathematicslessonsscoredanaverageof15pointslowerinmathematicsthandidstudentswhoreportedthatthisneveroralmostneverhappened(OECD,2023).Otherstudiessupportthishypothesis,havingfoundnegativerelationshipsbetween

tertiarystudentssmartphoneuseandtheiracademicperformance.AsummaryofthefindingsofseveralsuchstudiesispresentedbyAmezandBaert(2020).

ACTmathematicsscoreshavealsodeclinedovertime.In2019,forexample,theaveragescorewas20.4.In2023,itwas19.0(ACT,2023).UnlikePISAsciencescores,however,theACT

sciencescorehasdeclinedaswell(20.6in2019,19.6in2023).DeclinesinACTscoresappeartobesmallerthanthoseinPISAscoresbecauseofthedifferentscalesusedbythetwo

assessments.TheACTscorescalerangesfrom1to36,andadeclineof1or2pointsinan

averagescorecanbesubstantive.Incomparison,PISAscorestheoreticallyhavenominimumormaximumbutinsteadarescaledtofitdistributionsthatareapproximatelynormal,withmeansaround500scorepointsandstandarddeviationsaround100scorepoints(OECD,2023).

Therefore,similar1-or2-pointdeclinesinPISAscoresgenerallyarenotsubstantive.

FactorsthatmighthavebeenexacerbatedbytheCOVID-19pandemic,suchasmathematicsanxiety,mightalsoberelatedtothemathematicsperformancedecline.Mathematicsanxietyhasbeenshowntoberelatedbothtodecreasedmathematicsperformance(Richardson&

Suinn,1972)andtoaffectedself-confidenceandclearthinkingwhenfacingamathematicsproblem,especiallyforfemalehighschoolstudents(Escalera-Chávezetal.,2017).

TheCOVID-19pandemicwasfoundtoberelatedtoasignificantincreaseinthelevelof

mathematicsanxietyinasampleofcollegestudents(Soysaletal.,2022).Inaddition,limited

accesstotechnologyandaninabilitytocommunicateadequatelywithinstructorswererelated

toincreasesinmathematicsanxietyamongcollegestudentsfollowingapandemic-driven

emergencytransitiontoremotelearning(Laniusetal.,2022).PerhapsthepandemiccontributedinasimilarwaytoincreasedmathematicsanxietyandtobothACTandPISAmathematics

scoredeclinesamonghighschoolstudents.

Thepurposeofthisstudywastoexplore1)therelationshipbetweentechnologydistractionsinmathematicsclasses,specificallythoseresultingfromtheuseofsmartphones,and

performanceontheACTmathematicstestand2)therelationshipbetweenmathematicsanxietyandperformanceontheACTmathematicstest.Inaddition,todeterminewhethersmartphonedistractionsandanxietymightberelatedtoperformanceinsubjectsotherthanmathematics,

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20253

CT

?2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435

thisstudyexploredrelationshipsamongsmartphonedistractionsinscienceclasses,scienceanxiety,andperformanceontheACTsciencetest.

Thedataforthisstudywereobtainedfromasampleofhighschoolstudentswhohadtaken

eithertheDecember2023ortheFebruary2024nationalACTtestorhadregisteredfor,butnotyettaken,theApril2024test.InApril2024,thesestudentswereaskedseveralquestions

designedtoevaluate1)howdistractedtheywerebytheirownorotherstudentssmartphonesduringmathematicsandscienceclassesand2)thedegreeoftheirmathematicsandscienceanxiety.

1

Itwashypothesizedthatthedatawouldindicateanegativerelationshipbetweenmathematics

anxietyandACTmathematicsperformance(i.e.,higherlevelsofanxietywouldberelatedto

lowerlevelsofperformance)andasimilarnegativerelationshipbetweensmartphone

distractionsinmathematicsclassesandACTmathematicsperformance(i.e.,higherlevelsof

distractionwouldberelatedtolowerlevelsofperformance).Similarfindingswereanticipatedforscience.

Findings

ACTMathematicsPerformanceandSmartphoneDistractionsinMathematicsClasses

DistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones

Studentswereaskedhowoftentheyweredistractedduringtheirmathematicsclassesbytheirownsmartphones.MeanACTmathematicsscoresdecreasedasthefrequencyofsmartphonedistractionsincreased(Figure1).

2

Forexample,themeanscoreofstudentswhoreportedthattheywereneveroralmostneverdistractedbytheirphones(22.8)washigherthanthatof

studentswhoreportedthattheywerealmostalwaysdistractedbytheirphones(20.3).Analphalevelof.01wasusedinthisstudyforstatisticaltestsofdifferencesbetweenmeanscores.

Althoughthedifferencebetweenthesetwomeanscoreswasnotsignificantaccordingtothiscriterion(q=3.10,p=.0104),thepatternofdecreasesdisplayedbythemeanssuggeststhattherewasanegativerelationshipbetweenstudentsperformanceontheACTmathematicstestandthefrequencyofdistractionsfromtheirsmartphonesduringmathematicsclasses.A

regressionanalysis,whichisdescribedlaterinthissection,confirmedthis.

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20254

Figure1.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones

MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)

26

25

24

23

22

21

20

19

18

22.8

22.1

21.6

20.3

Neveroralmostnever

(n=4,119)

SometimesOften

(n=2,339)(n=561)

HowOftenDistractedByYourSmartphone

Almostalways

(n=258)

DistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents

Studentswereaskedhowoftentheyweredistractedduringtheirmathematicsclassesbyotherstudents’smartphones.Similartowhatwasobservedfordistractionsfromstudents’own

smartphones,meanACTmathematicsscorestendedtodecreaseasthefrequencyof

distractionsfromotherstudents’smartphonesincreased(Figure2).Anunexpectedfindingwasthatthemeanofthe“almostalways”levelwassomewhatlargerthanthatofthe“often”level

(20.5versus19.8,respectively;thedifferencebetweenthesemeanswasnotstatistically

significant;q=0.83,p=.8401).Apossibleexplanationisthatstudentswhosaidthattheywerealmostalwaysdistractedbyotherstudents’smartphonesduringmathematicsclasseshad

adjustedwelltofrequentdistractionsandthuswereabletolearnmathematicalconceptsaswellasthosestudentswhoreportedsomewhatfewerdistractions.Itisimportanttokeepinmindthattheinterpretationoffindingsforthisdistractionlevelmightbelimitedbecauseofitsrelatively

smallsamplesize(n=183).

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20255

Figure2.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents

MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)

26

25

24

23

22

21

20

19

18

22.8

22.4

20.5

19.8

NeveroralmostneverSometimesOftenAlmostalways

(n=4,810)(n=1,738)(n=451)(n=183)

HowOftenDistractedByOtherStudents'Smartphones

Anotherexplanationisthattherewasadisproportionatenumberofhigh-achievingstudentsinthe“almostalways”distractionlevelandthattheywereabletofocusinmathematicsclasses

irrespectiveofthefrequencyofsmartphonedistractions.Thispossibilityissupportedbyastudythatexaminedtheacademicperformanceofhighschoolstudentsfollowingmobilephonebansintheirschools.Thatstudy’sfindingssuggestedboththathigh-achievingstudentswereabletofocusintheclassroomwhethermobilephoneswerebannedornotandthattheywere

unaffectedbyanynegativeimpactsofmobilephoneuse(Beland&Murphy,2016).However,thecurrentstudyfoundnoevidencethattherewasadisproportionatenumberofhighachieversinthe“almostalways”level(studentachievementlevelwasdeterminedbyenrollmentin

advancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinmathematics;thenumberofmathematicscoursestaken;andhighschoolGPA).

Incaseschool-widesmartphonebansweresomehowrelatedtothisfinding,theproportionsofstudentswhoseschoolshadbannedsmartphones(seeQuestion5intheSurveyInstrument

sectionofthetechnicalappendix)werecomparedacrossdistractionlevels.Theproportionsdidnotdiffersignificantly.Moreover,itwasfoundinaregressionmodelforpredictingACT

mathematicsperformance—amodelthatincludedavariablefordistractionsfromother

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20256

students’smartphones,avariableforschool-widesmartphonebans,andvariablesrelatedtostudentbackground—thattheregressioncoefficientforschool-widesmartphonebanswasnotstatisticallysignificant(b=?0.71,t=?2.45,p=.0144).

3

ThissuggeststhatsmartphonebanswerenotrelatedtoACTmathematicsperformanceanddistractionsfromotherstudents’

smartphoneswhenstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.

Thereisanotherpossibilitywemightconsider.Perhapsthisunexpectedfindinghassomethingtodowiththescaleusedforthisquestion.Thatis,ifstudentshaddifficultydistinguishing

betweenthe“often”and“almostalways”levels,thenthedifferencebetweenthemeansfor

theselevelscouldbetheresultofmeasurementerror.However,asimilarfindingdidnotoccurforthequestionaboutdistractionsfromstudents’ownsmartphonesinmathematicsclasses,whichusedthesamescale.Thus,measurementerrorseemsanunlikelyexplanation.

ExpectedDecreasesinACTMathematicsScoreasaFunctionofSmartphoneDistractionsinMathematicsClassesandStudentBackgroundVariables

AregressionanalysiswasusedtofurtherexploretherelationshipbetweenACTmathematicsscoreandsmartphonedistractions.ACTmathematicsscorewasmodeledasafunctionofthefrequencyofdistractionsinmathematicsclassesfromeitherstudents’ownsmartphonesorthesmartphonesofotherstudents,plusseveralstudentbackgroundvariablesknowntoberelatedtoACTmathematicsscore.Thesevariablesincludedgender,race/ethnicity,familyincome

category,highschoolGPA,numberofmathematicscoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinmathematics.

FindingsoftheregressionanalysisindicatedthattherewouldbeanexpecteddecreaseinACTmathematicsscoreofapproximately?0.42scalescorepointsforeachunitchangeinlevelof

distractionfromastudent’sownsmartphonewhenstudentbackgroundvariableswere

controlledfor.Forexample,ifthelevelofdistractionfromastudent’ssmartphonewereto

increasefrom“neveroralmostnever”to“almostalways,”anincreaseofthreeunits,thenthe

expecteddecreaseinACTmathematicsscorewouldbe?1.26scalescorepoints(b=?0.42,t=?2.59,p=.0097;seeTableA2,Model1,inthetechnicalappendix).

TheexpecteddecreaseinACTmathematicsscorerelatedtodistractionsfromthesmartphonesofotherstudentswaslargerthanthatrelatedtodistractionsfromstudents’ownsmartphones:approximately?0.70scalescorepointsforeachunitchangeinlevelofdistraction.Forexample,ifthelevelofdistractionfromotherstudents’smartphonesweretoincreasefrom“neveror

almostnever”to“almostalways,”thentheexpectedmathematicsscoredecreasewouldbe?2.10scalescorepoints(b=?0.70,t=?4.11,p<.0001;TableA2,Model2).Clearly,thesefindingsindicateanegativerelationshipbetweensmartphonedistractionsandACT

mathematicstestperformance,evenwhentheeffectsofimportantstudentbackgroundvariablesarecontrolledfor.

ACTMathematicsPerformanceandMathematicsAnxiety

Toevaluatetheextentoftheirmathematicsanxiety,studentswereadministeredthe

AbbreviatedMathAnxietyScale(AMAS;Hopkoetal.,2003).TheAMASconsistsofnineitems,

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20257

fiveofwhichmeasureanxietyrelatedtolearningmathematics(LMA)andfourofwhichmeasuremathematicsevaluationanxiety(MEA).TheLMAsubscalescorerangesfrom5to25,andtheMEAsubscalescorerangesfrom4to20.AdditionalinformationabouttheAMAScanbefoundintheSurveyInstrumentsectionofthetechnicalappendix.

LearningMathematicsAnxiety

MeanACTmathematicsscoresdecreasedasthelevelofanxietyrelatedtolearning

mathematicsincreased(Figure3).Forexample,studentswithrelativelylowanxiety(i.e.,an

LMAsubscalescoreof5or6,whichwasthebottomquarteroftheLMAscoredistributionforstudentsinthisstudy)hadameanmathematicsscoreof24.6,whereasstudentswithrelativelyhighanxiety(i.e.,anLMAsubscalescorebetween13and25,thetopquarter)hadamean

mathematicsscoreof19.2.Thedifferencebetweenthesemeanswasstatisticallysignificant(q=14.87,p<.0001,d=1.01).

4

Figure3.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyLevelofLearningMathematicsAnxiety

MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)

26

25

24

23

22

21

20

19

18

24.6

23.7

22.4

19.2

5–6

(n=1,972)

7–8

(n=1,146)

9–12

(n=1,620)

13–25

(n=1,338)

LearningMathematicsAnxietySubscaleScore

MathematicsEvaluationAnxiety

Asimilar,negativerelationshiptothatbetweenmathlearninganxietyandACTmathematicsperformancewasobservedforanxietyrelatedtomathematicsevaluation.MeanACT

mathematicsscoresdecreasedasevaluationanxietyincreased,witha3.7scalescorepoint

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20258

CT

?2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435

differenceinmeanscores(q=8.62,p<.0001,d=0.65)betweenstudentswithrelativelylowanxiety(i.e.,anMEAsubscalescorebetween4and10,thebottomquarteroftheMEAscoredistribution)andthosewithrelativelyhighanxiety(i.e.,anMEAsubscalescoreof19or20,thetopquarter;Figure4).

Figure4.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyLevelofMathematicsEvaluationAnxiety

MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)

26

25

24

23

22

21

20

19

18

24.3

23.0

22.1

20.6

410

(n=1,561)

11141518

(n=1,544)(n=1,958)

MathematicsEvaluationAnxietySubscaleScore

1920

(n=1,063)

ExpectedDecreasesinACTMathematicsScoreasaFunctionofMathematicsAnxietyandStudentBackgroundVariables

MultiplelinearregressionwasusedtomodelACTmathematicsscoreasafunctionof

mathematicsanxiety(LMAsubscaleorMEAsubscale)andstudentbackgroundvariables

relatedtoperformanceonthistest(gender,race/ethnicity,familyincomecategory,highschoolGPA,numberofmathematicscoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinmathematics).Thefindingsindicatedthattherewouldbeanexpecteddecreaseinmathematicsscoreofapproximately?0.32scalescore

pointsforeachunitincreaseinLMAsubscalescore.Forexample,ifastudentslevelofLMAweretoincreasefrom8(themedianscoreforstudentsinthisstudy)to12(the75thpercentile),thentheexpecteddecreaseinmathematicsscorewouldbe?1.28scalescorepoints(b=

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20259

CT

?2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435

?0.32,t=?9.75,p<.0001;TableA2,Model3),evenwhenstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.

MultipleregressionfindingsformathematicsevaluationanxietyindicatedanegativerelationshipbetweenthismeasureandACTmathematicsperformance.ForeachunitincreaseinMEA,

therewasanexpecteddecreaseinmathematicsscoreofapproximately?0.24scalescorepoints(b=?0.24,t=?7.22,p<.0001;TableA2,Model4)whengender,race/ethnicity,andotherstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.

ACTSciencePerformanceandSmartphoneDistractionsinScienceClasses

TheprimarygoalofthisstudywastoexaminetherelationshipsbetweenACTmathematicsscoresandtwofactors:smartphonedistractionsinmathematicsclassesandmathematicsanxiety.Anothergoalwastodeterminewhethersmartphonedistractionsandanxietywererelatedtoperformanceinsubjectsotherthanmathematics.ThissectionpresentsfindingsofnegativerelationshipsbetweenACTscienceperformanceandtwofactors:smartphone

distractionsinscienceclassesandscienceanxiety.

5

DistractionsinScienceClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones

Asthefrequencyofdistractionsinscienceclassesincreased,ACTsciencescoresdecreasedonaverage.Forexample,themeansciencescoreofstudentswhoreportedthattheywere

neveroralmostneverdistractedbytheirownsmartphonesinscienceclasseswaslargerthanthatofstudentswhoreportedthattheywerealmostalwaysdistractedinthismanner(23.6

versus21.6,respectively;Figure5).However,thesemeansdidnotdiffersignificantlyfromeachother(q=2.80,p=.0266).

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March202510

CT

?2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435

Figure5.MeanACTScienceScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinScienceClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones

26

MeanACTScienceScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)

25

24

23

22

21

20

19

18

23.623.423.3

21.6

Neveroralmostnever

(n=3,910)

Sometimes

(n=2,204)

Often

(n=750)

Almostalways

(n=233)

HowOftenDistractedByYourSmartphone

DistractionsinScienceClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents

MeanACTsciencescorestendedtodecreaseasthefrequencyofdistractionsfromthe

smartphonesofotherstudentsinscienceclassesincreased.Forexample,themeanscience

scoreofstudentswhoreportedarelativelylowfrequencyofsmartphonedistractionwashigherthanthatofstudentswhoreportedarelativelyhighfrequency(meanfor“neveroralmostnever”level=23.5versus22.4for“almostalways”level;Figure6).However,thedifferencebetweenthesemeanswasnotstatisticallysignificant(q=1.35,p=.5295).

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March202511

Figure6.MeanACTScienceScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinScienceClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents

MeanACTScienceScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)

26

25

24

23

22

21

20

19

18

23.8

23.5

22.7

22.4

NeveroralmostneverSometimesOftenAlmostalways

(n=4,672)(n=1,686)(n=501)(n=213)

HowOftenDistractedByOtherStudents'Smartphones

Themeansciencescoreofstudentsinthe“sometimes”levelwasslightlyhigherthanthatof

studentsinthe“neveroralmostnever”level(23.8versus23.5,respectively;thedifference

betweenthesemeanswasnotstatisticallysignificant;q=0.88,p=.8153).Thehypothesis

proposedbyBelandandMurphy(2016)thathigh-achievingstudentsareabletofocusinthe

classroomandareunaffectedbyanynegativeimpactsofmobilephoneusewasconsideredasapossibleexplanationforthisunexpectedfinding.Severalmeasuresofstudentachievement

wereexaminedacrosssmartphonedistractionlevels.Itwasfoundthatthepercentageof

studentsenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinthenatural

sciences;thenumberofnaturalsciencescoursestaken;andhighschoolGPAwerecomparableacrosslevels.Therefore,itisunlikelythatthedifferencesinmeanACTsciencescoresacrossthedistractionlevelswererelatedtodifferencesinproportionsofhigh-achievingstudents.

School-widesmartphonebansdidnotappeartoberelatedtothisunexpectedfindingeither.

Theproportionsofstudentswhoseschoolshadimplementedsmartphonebansdidnotdiffersignificantlyacrossdistractionlevels.Inaddition,aregressionanalysisindicatedthat

smartphonebanswerenotsignificantlyrelatedtoACTsciencescore(b=一0.46,t=一1.51,p=.1313)whilecontrollingforsmartphonedistractionsinscienceclassesandstudentbackground

ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March202512

variables(gender,race/ethnicity,familyincomecategory,highschoolGPA,numberofnaturalsciencescoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,

accelerated,orhonorscoursesinthenaturalsciences).

ExpectedDecreasesinACTScienceScoreasaFunctionofSmartphoneDistractionsinScienceClassesandStudentBackgroundVariables

RegressionmodelsweredevelopedtofurtherexaminetherelationshipbetweenACTscienceperformanceandsmartphonedistractionswhilecontrollingforgender,race/ethnicity,family

incomecategory,highschoolGPA,numberofnaturalsciencescoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinthenatural

sciences.Unliketheregressioncoefficientsforthemathematicsmodels,however,the

regressioncoefficientsforthesciencemodelswerenotstatisticallysignificant(b=一0.20,t=

一1.12,p=.2617fordistractionsfromstudents’ownsmartphonesandb=一0.07,t=一0.37,p=.7092fordistractionsfromthesmartphonesofothers),thusprecludingacceptableestimationofdecreasesinACTsciencescorethatwouldbeexpectedforunitchangesinthesefactors.

Moreover,thesefindingssuggestthattherelationshipsbetweenACTscienceperformanceandsmartphonedistractionsinscienceclasseswerestatisticallyweakerthanthosebetweenACTmathematicsperformanceandsmartphonedistractionsinmathematicsclasses.

ACTSciencePerformanceandScienceAnxiety

Tomeasurestudents’levelsofscienceanxiety,theywereadministeredtheAbbreviated

ScienceAnxietyScale(ASAS;Megreyaetal.,2021),whichisadaptedfromthemodified

AbbreviatedMathAnxietyScale(m-AMAS;Careyetal.,2017).Them-AMASisadaptedfromtheAMAS.IncreatingtheASAS,theauthorsadjustedthem-AMASbyreplacingmathematics-relatedwordingwithscience-relatedwording.LiketheAMAS,thenine-itemASASyieldstwo

subscalescores,oneforlearningscienceanxiety(LSA;5items,withscoresrangingfrom5to25)andoneforscienceevaluationanxiety(SEA;4items,withscoresrangingfrom4to20).AdditionalinformationabouttheASAScanbefoundintheSurveyInstrumentsectionofthetechnicalappendix.

LearningScienceAnxiety

HighermeanACTsciencescoreswererelatedt

溫馨提示

  • 1. 本站所有資源如無特殊說明,都需要本地電腦安裝OFFICE2007和PDF閱讀器。圖紙軟件為CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.壓縮文件請下載最新的WinRAR軟件解壓。
  • 2. 本站的文檔不包含任何第三方提供的附件圖紙等,如果需要附件,請聯系上傳者。文件的所有權益歸上傳用戶所有。
  • 3. 本站RAR壓縮包中若帶圖紙,網頁內容里面會有圖紙預覽,若沒有圖紙預覽就沒有圖紙。
  • 4. 未經權益所有人同意不得將文件中的內容挪作商業或盈利用途。
  • 5. 人人文庫網僅提供信息存儲空間,僅對用戶上傳內容的表現方式做保護處理,對用戶上傳分享的文檔內容本身不做任何修改或編輯,并不能對任何下載內容負責。
  • 6. 下載文件中如有侵權或不適當內容,請與我們聯系,我們立即糾正。
  • 7. 本站不保證下載資源的準確性、安全性和完整性, 同時也不承擔用戶因使用這些下載資源對自己和他人造成任何形式的傷害或損失。

評論

0/150

提交評論