




版權說明:本文檔由用戶提供并上傳,收益歸屬內容提供方,若內容存在侵權,請進行舉報或認領
文檔簡介
1、 Literature Review In this thesis, I will study the characteristics of the conversational mechanism of repair in Chinese conversational discourse. To that end, it is necessary to conduct a review of the relevant literature on conversational repair. I shall start with an elaboration of the notion of
2、“repair”, going on to researches into the organization of conversational repair and conclude with the interdisciplinary and multi-linguistic application of “repair” research.1. From Correction to RepairAs a relatively new field in conversation analysis (CA), the proper study of the conversational ph
3、enomenon of repair didnt start until the publication of Schegloff et als seminal paper in 1977. Before that, there were only some sporadic discussions of the phenomenon under such generic headings as tongue slips (Laver 1973, see Schegloff 1977) and error correction (Jefferson 1975, see Schegloff 19
4、77). As a still often-used term, “correction”, “commonly understood to refer to the replacement of an error or mistake by what is correct” (Schegloff 1977: 363), not only limits research to a minority of the natural occurrences of repair but also misleads researchers about the nature of the trouble-
5、sources. The shift of focus was led by Schegloff et al (1977), whose study was an empirically based effort to examine the organization of repair as a set of ordered, but not equal possibilities. The phenomenon of correction was therefore proven part of a much wider picture, i.e. repair and the scope
6、 of discussion was greatly expanded from the mere correcting of some “hearable usually linguistic errors” (1977: 363) to all possible “practices for dealing with problems or troubles in speaking, hearing, and understanding the talk in conversation” (2000: 207), a definition given by Schegloff himsel
7、f some 20 years later. In deed, potential trouble-sources in conversation include not only correction of information, but also and more importantly replacement of inappropriate items or ambiguous anaphors, word search and clarification of the pragmatic function/understanding of a previous turn. Thes
8、e and many other occurrences may only be subsumed under the more general scope of repair. Incidentally, correction may not always be categorized under repair either, as is exemplified by the disagreement over the so-called “embedded correction” (Jefferson 1987) basically a covert form of other-corre
9、ction which Schegloff (2000) ruled out as not constituting a kind of repair. Equally important as the expansion in the scope of research was the change in the view of the trouble-sources that directly occasion the repair. According to Schegloff et al (1977), trouble-sources are not self-evident but
10、determined interactively by participants. In other words, all the segments in an utterance is, in theory, potential trouble-sources and often the existence of a trouble-source can only be evidenced by the actual mobilization of the practice of repair on the part of either the hearer or the speaker (
11、and sometimes both). It is worth noting that just as the status of a trouble-source is an uncertainty to be interactively determined, the actual need and proper protocol of its repair is not any more certain. This dynamic and interactive view of repair has proven rewarding in terms of revealing not
12、only its own mechanism but also other cognitive, social and psychological aspects of conversational discourse, as may be interestingly explained by such everyday wisdom: you dont know somethings at work until it goes wrong. 2. The organization of repair Many studies have been carried out with regard
13、 to the various dimensions of conversational repair itself, e.g. its classification, sites, forms and causes.Schegloff et al (1977) classified four interactional types of repair according to the subject(s) of initiation/repair, namely self/other-initiated self/other repair. This classification has b
14、een adopted by many researchers later, making it easier to tackle conversational data. Yet Geluykens (1994: 56) suggests, rightly I think, that this classification is in need of refinement as it is not always possible to draw a sharp boundary between self and other initiation. He found a sort of oth
15、er-prompted self-initiation, which underlines the interactive aspect of conversational discourse.Along with the interactional four-type classification, Schegloff et al (1977) proposed the unequal distribution of the four types. To be more exact, self-repair is preferred to other-repair and self-init
16、iation to other-initiation. It follows that the most favored type is self-initiated self-repair. Their claim was put forward with no statistical evidence so later researchers have discussed their empirical findings with reference to either or both of the two preferences. Many studies, including some
17、 based on data in languages other than English, are in support of the observation that self-repair is preferred, e.g. Geluykens (1994) and Ma (2007). Yet some remain doubtful as to the preference of self-initiation over other-initiation, e.g. Gaskell (1980), Schwartz (1980) and Gass & Varonis (1
18、985) (see Wang 2007). A strong objection to the preference of self-correction was put forward by Norrick (1991, see Jiang & Li 2003), whose data was collected from conversation in parent-child, teacher-student and NS-NNS contexts. After examining the organization of corrective exchanges in these
19、 contexts, he contended that the party abler to perform the correction not necessarily the speaker does it. Further, he dismissed the alleged preference as a sub-case which is only possible between adult native speakers, whose ability of repair is approximately equal. In other words, the absence of
20、such preference is the norm while the preference is a special case. Interestingly, Schegloff et al (1977) has also observed that other-correction “seems to be not as infrequent” and “appears to be one vehicle for socialization” in those contexts where someone not-yet-competent in a certain domain be
21、 it language facility or background information is involved (381). However, they further argued that this exception to the infrequency of other-correction is only a transitional stage and will be superseded by the preference of self-correction eventually. Joining in the heated discussion are Jiang &
22、amp; Li (2003), who also questioned the validity of Schegloffs claim about the preference for self-repair. They offered as proof the work of Norrick (1991) and Zhao (1996). The latter, on the basis of data obtained in academic seminars, of which other-repair takes up a remarkable proportion, suggest
23、ed that the option of self- or other-repair should take into account of the context, including the content of conversation and the respective social status of the participants (Jiang & Li 2003: 42). In their own survey, Jiang & Li (2003) calculated the frequencies of repair in two categories
24、 and found the preference of self-repair only existent in the category that included clearing up misunderstandings, word search or self-editing while in the correction of real errors, other-repair enjoys a bigger percentage of 60%. Therefore they blamed the mystery of the preference on the overly br
25、oad definition of repair put forward by Schegloff et al. Besides the interactional four-type classification, repair has been classified by other ways. In terms of the kind of trouble-spot being repaired, Levelt (1989, see Geluykens 1994:20) distinguishes between Error-repair and Appropriateness-repa
26、ir. Considering the temporal aspect of repair, there are immediate repairs and delayed repairs (Geluykens 1994: 22). There has also been in-depth discussion on the sites, or what is called the sequential environment for repair initiation and reparans (the repairing segment). A usual way of referring
27、 to the position of repair initiation is by reference to the turn where the trouble-source occurs. Schegloff et al (1977) found self-initiation mainly in three positions, namely the same turn as the trouble-source, the same turns transition place and the third-turn to the trouble-source turn; other-
28、initiation, on the other hand, was found mainly in the next turn (to the trouble-source turn). Levinson (1983, see Geluykens 1994) identified four similar opportunities, which are ordered with decreasing preference and most often used by either self- or other-initiation. In particular, Schegloff (20
29、00) elaborated the locus of other-initiation (OI) that occurs in positions other than the turn following the trouble-source turn. He suggested several interactional constraints that may be accountable for these somewhat deviant OIs, constraints related to the organization of repair, of turns or of t
30、urn-taking. In addition, he observed occasional delays in OIs which implies the speakers intention of “setting aside the understanding problem” (233) or assessing it later. This observation was of great relevance to the study of Wong, who examined a form of “delayed next turn repair initiation” in N
31、-NN English conversation and proposed that it might be accounted by the differences between native and non-native participants in their ways of social interaction more specifically, in the use of certain tokens and sequential organization in conversation. From the comparison between Schegloff (2000)
32、 and Wong (2000), it seems that the instantaneity and complexity of conversation spell danger for hasty generalization and due attention should be paid to minute differentiation. A case in point may be found in Schegloff (1997)s distinction between “third turn repair” and “third position repair”, bo
33、th of which occupies as a rule the turn subsequent to the turn following the trouble-source turn, hence “third”. Yet a closer look with a focus on sequential relevance will clear up the confusion of the two. While “third position repair” is usually self-repair in response to other-initiation in the
34、second turn, “third turn repair” is a kind of self-initiated self-repair separated from the trouble-source turn only by a not full-fledged turn of acknowledgement or irrelevant interpolation. Another dimension of conversational repair, i.e. its forms, has also received considerable academic attentio
35、n. Firstly, on the various forms of initiation, Schegloff (1979) distinguished between lexical and non-lexical initiation; Kuang (2001) specified five forms of repair initiation with decreasing extent of repetition of the trouble-source turn; Drew (1997) developed a sequential analysis of the use of
36、 open initiators (such as “pardon?”, “sorry?” and “what?”), in which specific forms of initiation are correlated with specific types of trouble sources.Secondly, the forms of the reparans (the repairing utterance) prove a complex issue, as researchers have found an undeniable relation between repair
37、 and syntax. For one thing, the four forms taken by same-turn reparans recycling, replacing, inserting and restarting (Schegloff 1979) may change the syntactic structure of the trouble-source turn. As it is, repair “can drastically change the syntactic form by subsuming, under another frame sentence
38、, the whole sentence being said or starting to be said” (Schegloff 1979: 280). This interaction between repair and syntax is partly responsible for the confusion of repair with other constructions, e.g. dislocations. In this interesting aspect, Geluykens (1994) explored intensively the mechanism of
39、right dislocation (RD), which often overlaps with anaphoric repair because of their similarity in syntactic characteristics, semantic relations and functions. Ma (2006) categorized RDs with repairing function in Chinese discourse (including a drama script) into the four interactional types of repair
40、. It is not easy to judge whether a RD is functioning as a repair and it is of great help to take into account the prosodic features, as Geluykens wisely and meticulously did.As a kind of repair which has received the widest attention, anaphoric repairs are mobilized by the following general causes,
41、 trouble-sources being their specific causes. These general causes may be: (1) the online nature of naturally occurring conversation (Biber et al, see Ma 2007); (2) the “discrepancy between the speakers assessment and the hearers actual state of knowledge” (Huang 1994: 213; also cf. Sacks & Sche
42、gloff ); (3) failure to satisfy concurrently two pragmatic principles, which are the Quantity- and Informativeness-principles according to Huang (1994) but which are the Economy- and Clarity-principles according to Geluykens (1994). It seems that the three causes are closely related rather conflicti
43、ng. For one thing, a balanced satisfaction of two pragmatic principles requires above all the speakers correct assessment of the hearers actual state of knowledge. Moreover, these causes have general implications for the causes of other kinds of repair.3. Towards a broader scopeThough the majority o
44、f empirical materials for the study of repair are drawn from English conversation, works have been done on talk-in-interaction in a broader range of languages and communities. These efforts have proven fruitful to some extent. A comparative study by Rieger (2003) found that the structural difference
45、 between English and German may have resulted in the different preferences of the form of repetition as self-repair strategies among English and German native speakers. Similarly, Fox et al (see Shen 2005: 39-40) proposed that the difference in the forms of repair in Japanese and English is partly c
46、aused by the difference in the syntax of the two languages. Ma (2007) examined repair strategies employed by native speakers of Mandarin Chinese and found a similar skewed distribution of the four interactional types among both literate and illiterate groups. Other studies on repair in Mandarin Chin
47、ese include a brief discussion of anaphoric repair by Huang (2000) and an inspiring survey of the classification, positioning and forms of repair and repair initiation by Jiang (2001) and a detailed examination of the forms of repair aimed at problems of production or understanding by Luo (2004). Mo
48、erman (1977, see Geluykens 1994: 20)s findings in a Thai conversational corpus also reinforced the claim of the preference for self-initiated self-repair. Meanwhile, long-due attention has been paid to the organization of repair in non-native discourse communities. The interest in repair strategies
49、of non-native English speakers in English conversation has been given a boost by the need to explore “the potential value of CA for the study of SLA through interaction” (Wong 244). Quantitative surveys were carried out by Wang (2007) and Chen & Pu (2007) among non-native English speakers in Chi
50、na. In both surveys, non-native speakers were found to favor the repair of errors in linguistic forms rather than improper expressions or inadequate information. The three researchers thus suggested that language teachers should place greater emphasis on communicative competence. Kaspers investigati
51、on in the ESL classroom is also an effort in this direction (see Shen 40). Hence it seems a justified effort for language learning and teaching to take a much closer look at non-native talk how it may go wrong and then be repaired. As conversation is a most common practice of interpersonal interacti
52、on and social communication, the organization of repair in conversation has also sparked interest in interdisciplinary research. Schegloff rightly pointed out that “at the organization of repair thought not exclusively here linguistics and sociology meet.” (Schegloff 1977: 381). Faerch& Kasper (
53、see Yao 2005) proposed that problematic utterance is a face-threatening act and accordingly, self-repair is a face-saving act. Similarly, Wong (2000) interpreted the non-native speakers ambiguous response (e.g. “oh”) to the native speaker as a “face-saving acknowledgment token” (263), which does not
54、 signal an adequate understanding of the preceding turn and which is often followed by other-initiation from the non-native speaker. Here, a sense of “nonnative-ness” seems to be at work. Surprisingly, even among native speakers themselves, the sense of “nonnative-ness” may also arise and membership
55、 categorizing may be under way. By analyzing German conversation, Egbert (2004) showed just exactly how “coparticipants engage in linguistic and regional membership categorizing in other-initiated repair sequences” (28). Both Wong and Egbert has shown the potential for CA methodology to be applied t
56、o research in intercultural and intra-cultural communications.Researchers in psycholinguistics and computational linguistics have also been interested in the classification of self-repair and the perception of repair through sound signals and syntactic analysis (cf. Yao 2005 and Shen 2005).Reference
57、sDrew, P. (1997). Open class repair initiators in response to sequential sources of troubles in conversation J. Journal of Pragmatics 28: 69-101.Egbert, M. (2004). Other-initiated repair and membership categorization some conversational events that trigger linguistic and regional membership categorization J. Journal of Pragmatics 36: 1467-1498.Geluykens, R. (1994). The pragmatics of discourse anaphora in Engl
溫馨提示
- 1. 本站所有資源如無特殊說明,都需要本地電腦安裝OFFICE2007和PDF閱讀器。圖紙軟件為CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.壓縮文件請下載最新的WinRAR軟件解壓。
- 2. 本站的文檔不包含任何第三方提供的附件圖紙等,如果需要附件,請聯系上傳者。文件的所有權益歸上傳用戶所有。
- 3. 本站RAR壓縮包中若帶圖紙,網頁內容里面會有圖紙預覽,若沒有圖紙預覽就沒有圖紙。
- 4. 未經權益所有人同意不得將文件中的內容挪作商業或盈利用途。
- 5. 人人文庫網僅提供信息存儲空間,僅對用戶上傳內容的表現方式做保護處理,對用戶上傳分享的文檔內容本身不做任何修改或編輯,并不能對任何下載內容負責。
- 6. 下載文件中如有侵權或不適當內容,請與我們聯系,我們立即糾正。
- 7. 本站不保證下載資源的準確性、安全性和完整性, 同時也不承擔用戶因使用這些下載資源對自己和他人造成任何形式的傷害或損失。
最新文檔
- 2024年09月廣東東莞市公立醫院高層次人才引進388人筆試歷年專業考點(難、易錯點)附帶答案詳解
- 文化用品租賃業務品牌推廣策略考核試卷
- 工廠企業顏色標識管理培訓
- 白酒與時尚與潮流文化考核試卷
- 2024年09月四川大學華西樂城醫院招聘18人筆試歷年專業考點(難、易錯點)附帶答案詳解
- 企業質量管理培訓
- 環保水性漆包裝考核試卷
- 2025裝載機買賣合同范本
- 2024年08月湖南長沙市婦幼保健院自主招聘工作人員考核筆試歷年專業考點(難、易錯點)附帶答案詳解
- 2025版合同:房屋建筑招標投標合同(監理邀請書)
- 防性侵安全教育課件
- 《食品儀器分析技術》項目七質譜法及其在食品分析中的應用
- 北京市2024年中考歷史真題試卷(含答案)
- 職業技能大賽-鴻蒙移動應用開發賽初賽理論知識考試及答案
- 2024年全國高考日語試卷(新題型)(含答案與解析)
- 部編版六年級下冊《第14課 文言文二則》2024年同步練習卷
- 報銷單據明細表Excel模板
- 2024-2030年中國低空監視雷達行業市場發展趨勢與前景展望戰略研究報告
- 學習《吳軍閱讀與寫作》 (50講 )
- 12J003《室外工程圖集》
- JGJ196-2010建筑施工塔式起重機安裝、使用、拆卸安全技術規程
評論
0/150
提交評論